RESEARCH AND DESIGN COOPERATION 59 0 Are natural and built landmarks utilized to help give individual identity to dif- ferent clusters and different parts of large sites? (88) Design review questions included in important evidence-based design volumes (Cooper Marcus and Barnes, 1999) have no right or wrong answers; they sim— ply raise issues for discussion. By answering the questions, every member of the design team gains greater control over design decisions. This holds for design- ers as well as users and other clients. They all contribute to improving a design. Users gain a special additional benefit from design review questions. Assessing how well each question corresponds to their own lives makes apparent to them knowledge and experience they may not know they have. They are put in the position not of individual clients being asked to express personal desires and tastes but of knowledgeable participants whose ability to tailor the research to a particular situation is valuable to design. Post-Occupancy Evaluation (POE) Another occasion for designers and researchers to cooperate is after a building or other setting has been occupied—when it is in use. POEs answer questions like: What were the designers’ original intentions, and how did they try to implement them? What is there in the design that influenced use of the setting in ways the designers did not intend? How did position, expertise, and know- how of design-team members affect decisions during design? Answers to such questions can be used to reorganize design teams, improve designers’ control over the effects of future design decisions, and test theories on which design decisions are based. One problem in POE research is how to reconstruct design decisions. Investigators can better use POE results to improve the process of making design decisions in the future if they can identify and make visible the design decisions that led up to the setting being evaluated. If designers and researchers collaborated throughout the design process—programming, design, and construction—making the process visible is less of a problem; it is more of a problem if behavioral information was not made explicit during design. An explicit behavioral performance program and annotated plans on which designers have presented behavioral expectations provide a solid footing for evaluation researchers to reconstruct the behavioral components of a design process. For example, in the NIH cancer-treatment center designed with research, the program and annotated plans could be translated into testable hypotheses, as in Table 3-1 (Conway et al., 1977). The more inventive evaluators are, the more hypotheses they will gener- ate from any stated intention, and the more they will profit from their study. In a POE of a setting for which no behavioral program or written expla— nation of design decisions was prepared at the time decisions were made, investigators have greater difficulty reconstructing the how and why of deci- sions. The problem is further compounded when decisions were made by designers who no longer work for the firm, or by one of several unidentifiable